Top

Study explains how brain reacts to someone in pain

Researchers explored the possibility of whether altering activity in the tactile brain regions would alter people's willingness to help.

Washington: Helping other people in need is a foundation of our society. It is intuitive to believe that we help others because we empathically share their pain.

Neuroscience shows that when we see somebody in pain our brain activates tactile and emotional regions as if we ourselves were in pain.

Selene Gallo of the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience explored the possibility of whether altering activity in these tactile brain regions while witnessing the pain of others would alter people's willingness to help.

The results were of great importance to understand our social human nature and to find treatments for pathologies, like psychopath individuals.

To answer the question, researchers from the Social Brain Lab, led by Valeria Gazzola and Christian Keysers gave participants the opportunity to reduce the pain of a victim receiving a swat on her hand. They could reduce the pain by giving up money they could have otherwise taken home while their tactile brain activity was measured and altered.

The researchers used electroencephalography, a method to record the electrical activity of the brain, in healthy human participants. With this method, they found that the activity in tactile cortices increased when participants increased their donation.

Later they altered brain activity by using neuromodulation. Normally participants gave more money when the victim experienced more pain. But when interfering with tactile activity two related phenomena were observed: (a) people became less able to perceive in how much pain the other person was and (b) they no longer adapted their donations as appropriately to the needs of the other.

The results suggested that our tactile cortices, primarily evolved to perceive touch and pain on our body, have an important social function. They contributed to prosocial decision-making by helping to transform the sight of bodily harm into an accurate feeling for how much pain the victim experiences. This feeling was necessary to adapt our help to the needs of others.

Providing a link between empathy for pain and prosocial behaviour at the neural level was crucial to understand our social human nature and target pharmacological approaches to treat pathologies in which these mechanisms dysfunction, for example in psychopath individual or in children with callous-unemotional traits.

The findings from the study are published in the journal eLife.

Next Story